Research Article
BibTex RIS Cite

PHONOLOGICAL PROCESSING OF BILINGUALS AND LANGUAGE NEURAL NETWORKS

Year 2024, Volume: 9 Issue: 2, 1185 - 1208, 26.12.2024
https://doi.org/10.32321/cutad.1553753

Abstract

Advances in neuroimaging techniques and analytical methods have led to an increase in studies investigating the effects of bilingualism on human cognitive and brain systems. Although hundreds of studies in our age have addressed the processing of the components of the language system in the brain, explanations regarding the definition of the neural basis of language and speech are being updated and increasing rapidly. The idea that language components are activated in only one center of the brain during the production and perception of human speech is not correct. The regions known as subcortical neuroanatomical regions in the brain, which are responsible for the perception and production of the act of speech, are connected to each other by anterior (ventral) and posterior (dorsal) cortical pathways and undertake important tasks, primarily phonological processing, in both hemispheres of the brain. In this context, it can be said that there is a combinatoric process in the performance of tasks within the framework of the processing of the components of language. This combinatoric process is not fixed in a certain part of the brain, but occurs bilaterally and lateralized to the left. The processing of the phonological component in monolingual and bilingual individuals is a prominent topic in the field of neurolinguistics. It has long been assumed that the complex language network supporting language processing in adulthood is the result of a temporally long-term interaction of maturation and language exposure. Functional left lateralization and increased involvement of frontal structures are considered normal developmental processes of neurolinguistic specialization. Thanks to studies examining how the processing of the phonological component in the brain occurs in monolingual and bilingual individuals, it is now known that the phonological component is the earliest processed component of the language system in the brain. Although this component follows the same steps in the processing process in monolinguals and bilinguals, it is undeniable that it creates differences in the development of brain networks from an individual perspective. In this study, the perception and production of speech have been attempted to be explained based on the data of the researches in the subject and the neural basis of the phonological processing of bilinguals has been explained. This study aims to elucidate the cortical basis of phonological processing in bilinguals by focusing on EEG and functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of phonological perception. Although the results show differences between studies depending on stimulus characteristics, task demands, and participants' previous experience with D2 sounds, the literature suggests that brain regions such as dorsal auditory-motor, including the frontal and posterior cortices, are involved in phonological processing in D1 and D2. Furthermore, studies have shown that bilinguals generally show greater volumes of language structure components and pathways between brain areas compared to monolinguals.

References

  • Abutalebi, J. (2008). Neural aspects of second language representation and language control. Acta Psychologica, 128(3), 466-478.
  • Abutalebi, J., Della Rosa, P. A., Gonzaga, A. K. C., Keim, R., Costa, A., & Perani, D. (2013). The role of the left putamen in multilingual language production. Brain and Language, 125(3), 307-315.
  • Albareda‐Castellot, B., Pons, F., & Sebastián‐Gallés, N. (2011). The acquisition of phonetic categories in bilingual infants: New data from an anticipatory eye movement paradigm. Developmental Science, 14(2), 395-401.
  • Anderson, J. L., Morgan, J. L., & White, K. S. (2003). A statistical basis for speech sound discrimination. Language and Speech, 46(2-3), 155-182.
  • Andrews, S. (1997). The effect of orthographic similarity on lexical retrieval: Resolving neighborhood conflicts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4(4), 439-461.
  • Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(10), 829-839.
  • Beauvillain, C., & Grainger, J. (1987). Accessing interlexical homographs: Some limitations of a language-selective access. Journal of Memory and Language, 26(6), 658-672.
  • Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81(6), 1641-1660.
  • Bialystok, E. (2001). Bilingualism in Development: Language, Literacy, and Cognition. Cambridge University Press.
  • Bialystok, E., & Hakuta, K. (1994). In other words. New York: Basic Books.
  • Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., & Luk, G. (2012). Bilingualism: Consequences for mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(4), 240-250.
  • Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19(2), 290.
  • Brysbaert, M., Van Dyck, G., & Van de Poel, M. (1999). Visual word recognition in bilinguals: Evidence from masked phonological priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(1), 137.
  • Bull, R., & Lee, K. (2014). Executive functioning and mathematics achievement. Child Development Perspectives, 8(1), 36-41.
  • Burns, T. C., Yoshida, K. A., Hill, K., & Werker, J. F. (2007). The development of phonetic representation in bilingual and monolingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28(3), 455-474.
  • Calabria, M., Branzi, F. M., Marne, P., Hernández, M., & Costa, A. (2015). Age-related effects over bilingual language control and executive control. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(1), 65-78.
  • Carrasco‐Ortiz, H., Midgley, K. J., Frenck‐Mestre, C. ve diğ. (2012). Are phonological representations in bilinguals language specific? An ERP study on interlingual homophones. Psychophysiology, 49(4), 531-543.
  • Catani, M., Jones, D. K., & Ffytche, D. H. (2005). Perisylvian language networksof the human brain. Annals of Neurology, 57(1), 8-16.
  • Christoff, K., Prabhakaran, V., Dorfman, J., Zhao, Z., Kroger, J. K., Holyoak, K. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2001). Rostrolateral prefrontal cortex involvement in relational integration during reasoning. Neuroimage, 14(5), 1136-1149.
  • Colome A (2001). Lexical activation in bilinguals' speech production: language-specific or language-independent? Journal of Memory and Language 45:721–736.
  • Curtis, C. E., & D'Esposito, M. (2003). Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(9), 415-423.
  • De Groot AMB, Delmaar P, Lupker SJ (2000). The processing of interlexical homographs in translation recognition and lexical decision: support for non-selective access to bilingual memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section 53:397–428.
  • De Houwer, A. (1990). The Acquisition of Two Languages From Birth: A Case Study. Cambridge University Press.
  • Dehaene-Lambertz, G., & Gliga, T. (2004). Common neural basis for phoneme processing in infants and adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(8), 1375-1387.
  • Dijkstra T, De Bruijn ERA, Schriefers H, Ten Brinke S (2000a). More on interlingual homograph recognition: language mixing versus explictness of instruction. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 3:69–78.
  • Dijkstra T, Grainger J, Van Heuven WJB (1999). Recognition of cognates and interlingual homographs: the neglected role of phonology. Journal of Memory and Language 41:496–528.
  • Dijkstra T, Timmermans M, Schriefers H (2000b). On being blinded by your other language: effects of task demands on interlingual homograph recognition. Journal of memory and language 42:455–464.
  • Dijkstra T, Van Jaarsveld H, Ten Brinke S (1998). Interlingual homograph recognition: effects of task demands and language intermixing. Bilingualism: Language and cognition 1:51–66.
  • Dijkstra, T., & Van Heuven, W. J. (2002). The architecture of the bilingual word recognition system: From identification to decision. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5(3), 175-197.
  • Doctor, E. A., & Klein, D. (1992). Phonological processing in bilingual word recognition. In Advances In Psychology (Vol. 83, pp. 237-252). North-Holland.
  • Elmer, S., Hänggi, J., Meyer, M., & Jäncke, L. (2011). Differential language expertise related to white matter architecture in regions subserving sensory‐motor coupling, articulation, and interhemispheric transfer. Human Brain Mapping, 32(12), 2064-2074.
  • Elston-Güttler, K. E., Gunter, T. C., & Kotz, S. A. (2005). Zooming into L2: Global language context and adjustment affect processing of interlingual homographs in sentences. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(1), 57-70.
  • Fishman, J. A. (1968). Sociolinguistic perspective on the study of bilingualism.
  • Friederici, A. D. (2012a). The cortical language circuit: From auditory perception to sentence comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Science, 16(5), 262-268.
  • Friederici, A. D. (2012b). Language development and the ontogeny of the dorsal pathway. Frontiers in Evolutionary Neuroscience, 4(3), 1-7.
  • Friederici, A. D., & Alter, K. (2004). Lateralization of auditory language functions: a dynamic dual pathway model. Brain and Language, 89(2), 267-276.
  • Friederici, A. D., & Kotz, S. A. (2003). The brain basis of syntactic processes: functional imaging and lesion studies. Neuroimage, 20(1), 8-17.
  • Friederici, A. D., Balhmann, J., Heim, S., Schubotz, R. I., & Anwander, A. (2006). The brain differentiates human and non-human grammars: Functional localization and structural connectivity. PNAS, 103(7), 2458-2463.
  • Friederici, A. D., Pfeiffer, E., & Hahne, A. (1993). Event-related brain potentials during natural speech processing: effects of semantic, morphological and syntactic violatios. Cognitive Brain Research, 1(3), 183-192.
  • Garbin, G., Costa, A., Sanjuan, A., Forn, C., Rodriguez-Pujadas, A., Ventura, N. E. E. A., ... & Ávila, C. (2011). Neural bases of language switching in high and early proficient bilinguals. Brain and Language, 119(3), 129-135.
  • García-Pentón L, Pérez Fernández A, Iturria-Medina Y ve diğ. (2014). Anatomical connectivity changes in the bilingual brain. Neuroimage. 84:495–504.
  • Garcia-Sierra, A., Rivera-Gaxiola, M., Percaccio, C. R., Conboy, ve diğ. (2011). Bilingual language learning: An ERP study relating early brain responses to speech, language input, and later word production. Journal of Phonetics, 39(4), 546-557.
  • Gervain, J., & Mehler, J. (2010). Speech perception and language acquisition in the first year of life. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 191-218.
  • Golestani, N., Paus, T., & Zatorre, R. J. (2002). Anatomical correlates of learning novel speech sounds. Neuron, 35(5), 997-1010.
  • Gómez-Ruiz, M. I. (2010). Bilingualism and the brain: Myth and reality. Neurología (English Edition), 25(7), 443-452.
  • Gordon, Raymond G. (ed.). 2005. Ethnologue: Languages of The World (15th edn). Dallas, TX: SIL International.
  • Gow, D. W. (2012). The cortical organization of lexical knowledge: A dual lexicon model of spoken language processing. Brain and Language, 121(3), 273-288.
  • Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1(2), 67-81.
  • Grogan, A., Green, D. W., Ali, N., Crinion, J. T., & Price, C. J. (2009). Structural correlates of semantic and phonemic fluency ability in first and second languages. Cerebral Cortex, 19(11), 2690-2698.
  • Grosjean, F. (1982). Life With Two Languages: An Introduction To Bilingualism. Harvard University Press.
  • Herrmann, B., Maess, B., Hahne, A., Schröger, E., & Friederici, A. D. (2011). Syntactic and auditory spatial processing in the human temporal cortex: An meg study. Neuroimage, 57(2), 624-633.
  • Hickok, G. (2009). The functional neuroanatomy of language. Physics of Life Reviews, 6(3), 121-143.
  • Hickok, G., & Poeppel, D. (2004). Dorsal and ventral streams: A framework for understanding aspects of the functional anatomy of language. Cognition, 92(1-2), 67-99.
  • Hickok, G., & Poeppel, D. (2007). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(5), 393-402.
  • Hoff, E., Core, C., Place, S., Rumiche, R., Señor, M., & Parra, M. (2012). Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language, 39(1), 1-27.
  • Isel, F., Hahne, A., Maess, B., & Friederici, A. D. (2007). Neurodynamics of sentence interpretation: Erp evidence from french. Biological Psychology, 74, 337-346.
  • Jared D, Kroll JF (2001). Do bilinguals activate phonological representations in one or both of their languages when naming words? Journal of Memory and Language 44:21–31.
  • Jared, D., & Szucs, C. (2002). Phonological activation in bilinguals: Evidence from interlingual homograph naming. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5(3), 225-239.
  • Jared, D., Levy, B. A., & Rayner, K. (1999). The role of phonology in the activation of word meanings during reading: evidence from proofreading and eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128(3), 219.
  • Jasińska, K. K., & Petitto, L. A. (2014). Development of neural systems for reading in the monolingual and bilingual brain: New insights from functional near infrared spectroscopy neuroimaging. Developmental Neuropsychology, 39(6), 421-439.
  • Jusczyk, P. W., Friederici, A. D., Wessels, J. M., Svenkerud, V. Y., & Jusczyk, A. M. (1993). Infants′ sensitivity to the sound patterns of native language words. Journal of Memory and Language, 32(3), 402-420.
  • Kara, Ö., T. ve Şıhanlıoğlu, Ö. (2021). İki Dillillik, Çok dillilik ve Türleri. (ed. A. Karabulut, M. Tunagür.) İki Dilli Türk Çocuklarına Türkçe Öğretimi, Ankara: Akademisyen yayınevi, 31-54.
  • Kim, K. H., Relkin, N. R., Lee, K. M., & Hirsch, J. (1997). Distinct cortical areas associated with native and second languages. Nature, 388(6638), 171-174.
  • Kim, K. H., Relkin, N. R., Lee, K. M., & Hirsch, J. (1997). Distinct cortical areas associated with native and second languages. Nature, 388(6638), 171-174.
  • Kerkhofs, R., Dijkstra, T., Chwilla, D. J., & De Bruijn, E. R. (2006). Testing a model for bilingual semantic priming with interlingual homographs: RT and N400 effects. Brain Research, 1068(1), 170-183.
  • Klein, D., Mok, K., Chen, J. K., & Watkins, K. E. (2014). Age of language learning shapes brain structure: a cortical thickness study of bilingual and monolingual individuals. Brain and Language, 131, 20-24.
  • Kovelman, I., Baker, S. A., & Petitto, L. A. (2008). Age of first bilingual language exposure as a new window into bilingual reading development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11(2), 203-223.
  • Kovelman, I., Baker, S. A., & Petitto, L. A. (2008). Bilingual and monolingual brains compared: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of syntactic processing and a possible “neural signature” of bilingualism. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(1), 153-169.
  • Krizman, J., Marian, V., Shook, A., Skoe, E., & Kraus, N. (2012). Subcortical encoding of sound is enhanced in bilinguals and relates to executive function advantages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(20), 7877-7881.
  • Kuhl, P. K. (2004). Early language acquisition: cracking the speech code. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(11), 831-843.
  • Kuhl, P. K., Stevens, E., Hayashi, A. ve diğ. (2006). Infants show a facilitation effect for native language phonetic perception between 6 and 12 months. Developmental Science, 9(2), F13-F21.
  • Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1984). Brain potentials during reading reflect Word expectancy and semantic association. Nature, 307(5947), 161-163.
  • Lee, K., Ng, S. F., Bull, R., Pe, M. L., & Ho, R. H. M. (2011). Are patterns important? An investigation of the relationships between proficiencies in patterns, computation, executive functioning, and algebraic word problems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(2), 269.
  • Lemhofer K, Dijkstra T (2004). Recognizing cognates and interlingual homographs: effects of code similarity in language specific and generalized lexical decision. Journal of Memory and Language 32:533–550.
  • Leroy, F., Glasel, H., Dubois, J., Hertz-Pannier, L., Thirion, B., Mangin, J. F., & Dehaene-Lambertz, G. (2011). Early maturation of the linguistic dorsal pathway in human infants. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(4), 1500-1506.
  • Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., Loring, D. W., & Fischer, J. S. (2004). Neuropsychological Assessment. Oxford University Press, USA.
  • Li, P., Legault, J., & Litcofsky, K. A. (2014). Neuroplasticity as a function of second language learning: anatomical changes in the human brain. Cortex, 58, 301-324.
  • Linck, J. A., Hoshino, N., & Kroll, J. F. (2008). Cross-language lexical processes and inhibitory control. The Mental Lexicon, 3(3), 349-374.
  • Luk, G., Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., & Grady, C. L. (2011). Lifelong bilingualism maintains white matter integrity in older adults. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(46), 16808-16813.
  • Mahmoudzadeh, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Fournier ve diğ. (2013). Syllabic discrimination in premature human infants prior to complete formation of cortical layers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(12), 4846-4851.
  • Markman, E. M., & Wachtel, G. F. (1988). Children's use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words. Cognitive Psychology, 20(2), 121-157.
  • Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1987). Functional parallelism in spoken word-recognition. Cognition, 25(1-2), 71-102.
  • McCLelland, J. L., & Elman, J. L. (1986). The trace model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18(1), 1-86.
  • McLaughlin, B. (1995). Fostering second language development in young children: Principles and practices. Educational Practice Report: 14.
  • Mechelli, A., Crinion, J. T., Noppeney, U., O'Doherty, J., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S., & Price, C. J. (2004). Structural plasticity in the bilingual brain. Nature, 431(7010), 757-757.
  • Miller, E. K., & Wallis, J. D. (2009). Executive function and higher-order cognition: definition and neural substrates. Encyclopedia of Neuroscience, 4(99-104).
  • Minagawa-Kawai, Y., Mori, K., & Sato, Y. (2005). Different brain strategies underlie the categorical perception of foreign and native phonemes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(9), 1376-1385.
  • Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 8-14.
  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49-100.
  • Nosarti, C., Mechelli, A., Green, D. W., & Price, C. J. (2010). The impact of second language learning on semantic and nonsemantic first language reading. Cerebral Cortex, 20(2), 315-327.
  • Osterhout, L., & Holcomb, P. J. (1992). Event-related potentials and syntactic anomaly. Journal of Memory and Language, 31(4), 785-804.
  • Parker Jones, ve diğ. (2012). Where, when and why brain activation differs for bilinguals and monolinguals during picture naming and reading aloud. Cerebral Cortex, 22(4), 892-902.
  • Petitto, L. A., Berens, M. S., Kovelman, I. ve diğ. (2012). The “Perceptual Wedge Hypothesis” as the basis for bilingual babies’ phonetic processing advantage: New insights from fNIRS brain imaging. Brain and Language, 121(2), 130-143.
  • Petitto, L. A., Berens, M. S., Kovelman, I., Dubins, M. H., Jasinska, K., & Shalinsky, M. (2012). The “Perceptual Wedge Hypothesis” as the basis for bilingual babies’ phonetic processing advantage: New insights from fNIRS brain imaging. Brain and Language, 121(2), 130-143.
  • Ressel, V., Pallier, C., Ventura-Campos, N., Díaz, B., Roessler, A., Ávila, C., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2012). An effect of bilingualism on the auditory cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(47), 16597-16601.
  • Saur, D., Kreher, B. W., Schnell, S., Kümmerer, D., Kellmeyer, P., Vry, M.-S., et al. (2008). Ventral and dorsal pathways for language. PNAS, 105(46), 18035-18040.
  • Spivey, M. J., & Marian, V. (1999). Cross talk between native and second languages: Partial activation of an irrelevant lexicon. Psychological Science, 10(3), 281-284.
  • St Clair-Thompson, H. L., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Executive functions and achievements in school: Shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(4), 745-759.
  • Sundara, M., Polka, L., & Molnar, M. (2008). Development of coronal stop perception: Bilingual infants keep pace with their monolingual peers. Cognition, 108(1), 232-242.
  • Şıhanlıoğlu, Ö. (2021). An error analysis upon written expressions of Syrian late bilingual individuals in Turkey (Publication No. 658906) [Master thesis, Nevşehir Hacı Bektaş Veli Üniversitesi]. Yöktez. https://tez.yok.gov.tr/ UlusalTezMerkezi/tezSorguSonucYeni.jsp
  • Tan, L. H., Liu, H. L., Perfetti, C. A., Spinks, J. A., Fox, P. T., & Gao, J. H. (2001). The neural system underlying Chinese logograph reading. Neuroimage, 13(5), 836-846.
  • Uriel, W. (1953). Languages in contact. The Hague: Mouton, 1(1).
  • Van Heuven, W. J., Dijkstra, T., & Grainger, J. (1998). Orthographic neighborhood effects in bilingual word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 39(3), 458-483.
  • Verreyt, N., Woumans, E. V. Y., Vandelanotte, D., Szmalec, A., & Duyck, W. (2016). The influence of language-switching experience on the bilingual executive control advantage. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(1), 181-190.
  • Wagner, G., Koch, K., Reichenbach, J. R., Sauer, H., & Schlösser, R. G. (2006). The special involvement of the rostrolateral prefrontal cortex in planning abilities: an event-related fMRI study with the Tower of London paradigm. Neuropsychologia, 44(12), 2337-2347.
  • Weissberger, G. H., Gollan, T. H., Bondi, M. W., Clark, L. R., & Wierenga, C. E. (2015). Language and task switching in the bilingual brain: Bilinguals are staying, not switching, experts. Neuropsychologia, 66, 193-203.
  • Wong, B., Yin, B., & O’Brien, B. (2016). Neurolinguistics: Structure, function, and connectivity in the bilingual brain. Biomed Research International, 2016.
  • Wu, Y. J., & Thierry, G. (2010). Chinese–english bilinguals reading english hear chinese. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(22), 7646-7651.
  • Yip, V., & Matthews, S. (2007). Relative clauses in Cantonese-English bilingual children: Typological challenges and processing motivations. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 29(2), 277-300.
  • Zatorre, R. J., Belin, P., & Penhune, V. B. (2002). Structure and function of auditory cortex: Music and speech. Trends in Cognitive Science, 6(1), 37-46.
  • Zou, L., Abutalebi, J., Zinszer, B., Yan, X., Shu, H., Peng, D., & Ding, G. (2012). Second language experience modulates functional brain network for the native language production in bimodal bilinguals. NeuroImage, 62(3), 1367-1375.

İKİDİLLİLERDE SESBİLİMSEL İŞLEMLEME VE DİL SİNİR AĞLARI

Year 2024, Volume: 9 Issue: 2, 1185 - 1208, 26.12.2024
https://doi.org/10.32321/cutad.1553753

Abstract

Nörogörüntüleme teknikleri ve analitik yöntemlerdeki ilerlemeler, ikidilliliğin insanlarda bilişsel ve beyin sistemleri üzerindeki etkisini araştıran çalışmaların çoğalmasını sağlamıştır. Çağımızda yüzlerce çalışma, dil sisteminin bileşenlerinin beyindeki işlemlenişine değinse de dilin ve konuşmanın sinirsel temelinin tanımlanmasına ilişkin açıklamalar giderek güncellenmekte ve hızla artmaktadır. İnsan sesinin üretimi ve algılanması sürecinde, dil bileşenlerinin beyinin salt bir merkezinde aktivasyon gösterdiği düşüncesi doğru değildir. Beyinde korteksaltı nöroanatomik bölgeler olarak bilenen ve konuşma eyleminin algılanması ve üretiminden sorumlu olan bölgeler birbirilerine ön (ventral) ve arka (dorsal) kortikal yolaklarla bağlı olup beynin her iki yarıküresinde de sesbilimsel işlemleme başta olmak üzre önemli görevler üstelenmektedirler. Bu bağlamda dilin bileşenlerinin işlemlemesi çerçevesinde, görevlerin yerine getirilmesinde kombinatorik bir işleyiş olduğu söylenebilir. Bu kombinatorik işleyiş beynin belirli bir yerinde sabit olmayıp bilateral ve sola yanallaşmış bir şekilde gerçekleşmektedir. Tekdilli ve ikidilli bireylerde sesbilimsel bileşene ilişkin işlemleme, sinirdilbilimi alanında öne çıkan bir konudur. Uzun süredir, yetişkinlikte dil işlemeyi destekleyen karmaşık dil ağının, olgunlaşma ve dile maruz kalmanın geçici olarak uzun süreli bir etkileşimin sonucu olduğu varsayılmıştır. Fonksiyonel sol yanallaşma ve frontal yapıların artan katılımı, dil-sinir etkileşiminin normal gelişimsel seyri olarak kabul edilmektedir. Beyinde sesbilimsel bileşene ilişkin işlemleme sürecinin tekdilli ve ikidilli bireylerde nasıl gerçekleştiğini ele alan çalışmaların sayesinde, sesbilimsel bileşenin dil sisteminin beyinde en erken işlemlenen bileşen olduğu artık bilinmektedir. Bu bileşen tekdilli ve ikidilli işlemleme sürecinde aynı adımları izlese de bireysel açıdan beyin ağlarının gelişiminde farklılık yarattığı yadsınamaz. Bu çalışmayla konuşmanın algılanması ve üretimi, konu kapsamında alanyazınında yer alan araştırmaların verilerine dayanarak açıklanmaya çalışılmış ve ikidillilerin sesbilimsel işlemlemesinin nöral temelleri açıklanmıştır. Bu çalışma, sesbilimsel algının EEG ve fonksiyonel manyetik rezonans görüntüleme çalışmalarına odaklanarak, ikidillilerde sesbilimsel işlemlemenin kortikal temelini açıklamayı amaçlamaktadır. Sonuçlar, uyaran özelliklerine, görev taleplerine ve katılımcıların ikinci dil sesleriyle ilgili önceki deneyimlerine bağlı olarak çalışmalar arasında farklılığın olduğunu gösterse de alanyazın, birinci ve ikinci dildeki sesbilimsel işleme sırasında ön ve arka korteksler dâhilolmak üzere dorsal işitsel-motor gibi beyin bölgelerinin dâhil olduğunu ortaya koymaktadır. Ayrıca ikidillilerin genellikle tekdillilere görece dil yapılarının bileşenlerinde ve beyin alanları arasındaki bağlantı yollarında daha fazla hacim gösterdiği yapılan çalışmalarla ortaya konulmuştur.

References

  • Abutalebi, J. (2008). Neural aspects of second language representation and language control. Acta Psychologica, 128(3), 466-478.
  • Abutalebi, J., Della Rosa, P. A., Gonzaga, A. K. C., Keim, R., Costa, A., & Perani, D. (2013). The role of the left putamen in multilingual language production. Brain and Language, 125(3), 307-315.
  • Albareda‐Castellot, B., Pons, F., & Sebastián‐Gallés, N. (2011). The acquisition of phonetic categories in bilingual infants: New data from an anticipatory eye movement paradigm. Developmental Science, 14(2), 395-401.
  • Anderson, J. L., Morgan, J. L., & White, K. S. (2003). A statistical basis for speech sound discrimination. Language and Speech, 46(2-3), 155-182.
  • Andrews, S. (1997). The effect of orthographic similarity on lexical retrieval: Resolving neighborhood conflicts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4(4), 439-461.
  • Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(10), 829-839.
  • Beauvillain, C., & Grainger, J. (1987). Accessing interlexical homographs: Some limitations of a language-selective access. Journal of Memory and Language, 26(6), 658-672.
  • Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81(6), 1641-1660.
  • Bialystok, E. (2001). Bilingualism in Development: Language, Literacy, and Cognition. Cambridge University Press.
  • Bialystok, E., & Hakuta, K. (1994). In other words. New York: Basic Books.
  • Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., & Luk, G. (2012). Bilingualism: Consequences for mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(4), 240-250.
  • Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19(2), 290.
  • Brysbaert, M., Van Dyck, G., & Van de Poel, M. (1999). Visual word recognition in bilinguals: Evidence from masked phonological priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(1), 137.
  • Bull, R., & Lee, K. (2014). Executive functioning and mathematics achievement. Child Development Perspectives, 8(1), 36-41.
  • Burns, T. C., Yoshida, K. A., Hill, K., & Werker, J. F. (2007). The development of phonetic representation in bilingual and monolingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28(3), 455-474.
  • Calabria, M., Branzi, F. M., Marne, P., Hernández, M., & Costa, A. (2015). Age-related effects over bilingual language control and executive control. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(1), 65-78.
  • Carrasco‐Ortiz, H., Midgley, K. J., Frenck‐Mestre, C. ve diğ. (2012). Are phonological representations in bilinguals language specific? An ERP study on interlingual homophones. Psychophysiology, 49(4), 531-543.
  • Catani, M., Jones, D. K., & Ffytche, D. H. (2005). Perisylvian language networksof the human brain. Annals of Neurology, 57(1), 8-16.
  • Christoff, K., Prabhakaran, V., Dorfman, J., Zhao, Z., Kroger, J. K., Holyoak, K. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2001). Rostrolateral prefrontal cortex involvement in relational integration during reasoning. Neuroimage, 14(5), 1136-1149.
  • Colome A (2001). Lexical activation in bilinguals' speech production: language-specific or language-independent? Journal of Memory and Language 45:721–736.
  • Curtis, C. E., & D'Esposito, M. (2003). Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(9), 415-423.
  • De Groot AMB, Delmaar P, Lupker SJ (2000). The processing of interlexical homographs in translation recognition and lexical decision: support for non-selective access to bilingual memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section 53:397–428.
  • De Houwer, A. (1990). The Acquisition of Two Languages From Birth: A Case Study. Cambridge University Press.
  • Dehaene-Lambertz, G., & Gliga, T. (2004). Common neural basis for phoneme processing in infants and adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(8), 1375-1387.
  • Dijkstra T, De Bruijn ERA, Schriefers H, Ten Brinke S (2000a). More on interlingual homograph recognition: language mixing versus explictness of instruction. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 3:69–78.
  • Dijkstra T, Grainger J, Van Heuven WJB (1999). Recognition of cognates and interlingual homographs: the neglected role of phonology. Journal of Memory and Language 41:496–528.
  • Dijkstra T, Timmermans M, Schriefers H (2000b). On being blinded by your other language: effects of task demands on interlingual homograph recognition. Journal of memory and language 42:455–464.
  • Dijkstra T, Van Jaarsveld H, Ten Brinke S (1998). Interlingual homograph recognition: effects of task demands and language intermixing. Bilingualism: Language and cognition 1:51–66.
  • Dijkstra, T., & Van Heuven, W. J. (2002). The architecture of the bilingual word recognition system: From identification to decision. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5(3), 175-197.
  • Doctor, E. A., & Klein, D. (1992). Phonological processing in bilingual word recognition. In Advances In Psychology (Vol. 83, pp. 237-252). North-Holland.
  • Elmer, S., Hänggi, J., Meyer, M., & Jäncke, L. (2011). Differential language expertise related to white matter architecture in regions subserving sensory‐motor coupling, articulation, and interhemispheric transfer. Human Brain Mapping, 32(12), 2064-2074.
  • Elston-Güttler, K. E., Gunter, T. C., & Kotz, S. A. (2005). Zooming into L2: Global language context and adjustment affect processing of interlingual homographs in sentences. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(1), 57-70.
  • Fishman, J. A. (1968). Sociolinguistic perspective on the study of bilingualism.
  • Friederici, A. D. (2012a). The cortical language circuit: From auditory perception to sentence comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Science, 16(5), 262-268.
  • Friederici, A. D. (2012b). Language development and the ontogeny of the dorsal pathway. Frontiers in Evolutionary Neuroscience, 4(3), 1-7.
  • Friederici, A. D., & Alter, K. (2004). Lateralization of auditory language functions: a dynamic dual pathway model. Brain and Language, 89(2), 267-276.
  • Friederici, A. D., & Kotz, S. A. (2003). The brain basis of syntactic processes: functional imaging and lesion studies. Neuroimage, 20(1), 8-17.
  • Friederici, A. D., Balhmann, J., Heim, S., Schubotz, R. I., & Anwander, A. (2006). The brain differentiates human and non-human grammars: Functional localization and structural connectivity. PNAS, 103(7), 2458-2463.
  • Friederici, A. D., Pfeiffer, E., & Hahne, A. (1993). Event-related brain potentials during natural speech processing: effects of semantic, morphological and syntactic violatios. Cognitive Brain Research, 1(3), 183-192.
  • Garbin, G., Costa, A., Sanjuan, A., Forn, C., Rodriguez-Pujadas, A., Ventura, N. E. E. A., ... & Ávila, C. (2011). Neural bases of language switching in high and early proficient bilinguals. Brain and Language, 119(3), 129-135.
  • García-Pentón L, Pérez Fernández A, Iturria-Medina Y ve diğ. (2014). Anatomical connectivity changes in the bilingual brain. Neuroimage. 84:495–504.
  • Garcia-Sierra, A., Rivera-Gaxiola, M., Percaccio, C. R., Conboy, ve diğ. (2011). Bilingual language learning: An ERP study relating early brain responses to speech, language input, and later word production. Journal of Phonetics, 39(4), 546-557.
  • Gervain, J., & Mehler, J. (2010). Speech perception and language acquisition in the first year of life. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 191-218.
  • Golestani, N., Paus, T., & Zatorre, R. J. (2002). Anatomical correlates of learning novel speech sounds. Neuron, 35(5), 997-1010.
  • Gómez-Ruiz, M. I. (2010). Bilingualism and the brain: Myth and reality. Neurología (English Edition), 25(7), 443-452.
  • Gordon, Raymond G. (ed.). 2005. Ethnologue: Languages of The World (15th edn). Dallas, TX: SIL International.
  • Gow, D. W. (2012). The cortical organization of lexical knowledge: A dual lexicon model of spoken language processing. Brain and Language, 121(3), 273-288.
  • Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1(2), 67-81.
  • Grogan, A., Green, D. W., Ali, N., Crinion, J. T., & Price, C. J. (2009). Structural correlates of semantic and phonemic fluency ability in first and second languages. Cerebral Cortex, 19(11), 2690-2698.
  • Grosjean, F. (1982). Life With Two Languages: An Introduction To Bilingualism. Harvard University Press.
  • Herrmann, B., Maess, B., Hahne, A., Schröger, E., & Friederici, A. D. (2011). Syntactic and auditory spatial processing in the human temporal cortex: An meg study. Neuroimage, 57(2), 624-633.
  • Hickok, G. (2009). The functional neuroanatomy of language. Physics of Life Reviews, 6(3), 121-143.
  • Hickok, G., & Poeppel, D. (2004). Dorsal and ventral streams: A framework for understanding aspects of the functional anatomy of language. Cognition, 92(1-2), 67-99.
  • Hickok, G., & Poeppel, D. (2007). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(5), 393-402.
  • Hoff, E., Core, C., Place, S., Rumiche, R., Señor, M., & Parra, M. (2012). Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language, 39(1), 1-27.
  • Isel, F., Hahne, A., Maess, B., & Friederici, A. D. (2007). Neurodynamics of sentence interpretation: Erp evidence from french. Biological Psychology, 74, 337-346.
  • Jared D, Kroll JF (2001). Do bilinguals activate phonological representations in one or both of their languages when naming words? Journal of Memory and Language 44:21–31.
  • Jared, D., & Szucs, C. (2002). Phonological activation in bilinguals: Evidence from interlingual homograph naming. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5(3), 225-239.
  • Jared, D., Levy, B. A., & Rayner, K. (1999). The role of phonology in the activation of word meanings during reading: evidence from proofreading and eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128(3), 219.
  • Jasińska, K. K., & Petitto, L. A. (2014). Development of neural systems for reading in the monolingual and bilingual brain: New insights from functional near infrared spectroscopy neuroimaging. Developmental Neuropsychology, 39(6), 421-439.
  • Jusczyk, P. W., Friederici, A. D., Wessels, J. M., Svenkerud, V. Y., & Jusczyk, A. M. (1993). Infants′ sensitivity to the sound patterns of native language words. Journal of Memory and Language, 32(3), 402-420.
  • Kara, Ö., T. ve Şıhanlıoğlu, Ö. (2021). İki Dillillik, Çok dillilik ve Türleri. (ed. A. Karabulut, M. Tunagür.) İki Dilli Türk Çocuklarına Türkçe Öğretimi, Ankara: Akademisyen yayınevi, 31-54.
  • Kim, K. H., Relkin, N. R., Lee, K. M., & Hirsch, J. (1997). Distinct cortical areas associated with native and second languages. Nature, 388(6638), 171-174.
  • Kim, K. H., Relkin, N. R., Lee, K. M., & Hirsch, J. (1997). Distinct cortical areas associated with native and second languages. Nature, 388(6638), 171-174.
  • Kerkhofs, R., Dijkstra, T., Chwilla, D. J., & De Bruijn, E. R. (2006). Testing a model for bilingual semantic priming with interlingual homographs: RT and N400 effects. Brain Research, 1068(1), 170-183.
  • Klein, D., Mok, K., Chen, J. K., & Watkins, K. E. (2014). Age of language learning shapes brain structure: a cortical thickness study of bilingual and monolingual individuals. Brain and Language, 131, 20-24.
  • Kovelman, I., Baker, S. A., & Petitto, L. A. (2008). Age of first bilingual language exposure as a new window into bilingual reading development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11(2), 203-223.
  • Kovelman, I., Baker, S. A., & Petitto, L. A. (2008). Bilingual and monolingual brains compared: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of syntactic processing and a possible “neural signature” of bilingualism. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(1), 153-169.
  • Krizman, J., Marian, V., Shook, A., Skoe, E., & Kraus, N. (2012). Subcortical encoding of sound is enhanced in bilinguals and relates to executive function advantages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(20), 7877-7881.
  • Kuhl, P. K. (2004). Early language acquisition: cracking the speech code. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(11), 831-843.
  • Kuhl, P. K., Stevens, E., Hayashi, A. ve diğ. (2006). Infants show a facilitation effect for native language phonetic perception between 6 and 12 months. Developmental Science, 9(2), F13-F21.
  • Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1984). Brain potentials during reading reflect Word expectancy and semantic association. Nature, 307(5947), 161-163.
  • Lee, K., Ng, S. F., Bull, R., Pe, M. L., & Ho, R. H. M. (2011). Are patterns important? An investigation of the relationships between proficiencies in patterns, computation, executive functioning, and algebraic word problems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(2), 269.
  • Lemhofer K, Dijkstra T (2004). Recognizing cognates and interlingual homographs: effects of code similarity in language specific and generalized lexical decision. Journal of Memory and Language 32:533–550.
  • Leroy, F., Glasel, H., Dubois, J., Hertz-Pannier, L., Thirion, B., Mangin, J. F., & Dehaene-Lambertz, G. (2011). Early maturation of the linguistic dorsal pathway in human infants. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(4), 1500-1506.
  • Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., Loring, D. W., & Fischer, J. S. (2004). Neuropsychological Assessment. Oxford University Press, USA.
  • Li, P., Legault, J., & Litcofsky, K. A. (2014). Neuroplasticity as a function of second language learning: anatomical changes in the human brain. Cortex, 58, 301-324.
  • Linck, J. A., Hoshino, N., & Kroll, J. F. (2008). Cross-language lexical processes and inhibitory control. The Mental Lexicon, 3(3), 349-374.
  • Luk, G., Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., & Grady, C. L. (2011). Lifelong bilingualism maintains white matter integrity in older adults. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(46), 16808-16813.
  • Mahmoudzadeh, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Fournier ve diğ. (2013). Syllabic discrimination in premature human infants prior to complete formation of cortical layers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(12), 4846-4851.
  • Markman, E. M., & Wachtel, G. F. (1988). Children's use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words. Cognitive Psychology, 20(2), 121-157.
  • Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1987). Functional parallelism in spoken word-recognition. Cognition, 25(1-2), 71-102.
  • McCLelland, J. L., & Elman, J. L. (1986). The trace model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18(1), 1-86.
  • McLaughlin, B. (1995). Fostering second language development in young children: Principles and practices. Educational Practice Report: 14.
  • Mechelli, A., Crinion, J. T., Noppeney, U., O'Doherty, J., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S., & Price, C. J. (2004). Structural plasticity in the bilingual brain. Nature, 431(7010), 757-757.
  • Miller, E. K., & Wallis, J. D. (2009). Executive function and higher-order cognition: definition and neural substrates. Encyclopedia of Neuroscience, 4(99-104).
  • Minagawa-Kawai, Y., Mori, K., & Sato, Y. (2005). Different brain strategies underlie the categorical perception of foreign and native phonemes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(9), 1376-1385.
  • Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 8-14.
  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49-100.
  • Nosarti, C., Mechelli, A., Green, D. W., & Price, C. J. (2010). The impact of second language learning on semantic and nonsemantic first language reading. Cerebral Cortex, 20(2), 315-327.
  • Osterhout, L., & Holcomb, P. J. (1992). Event-related potentials and syntactic anomaly. Journal of Memory and Language, 31(4), 785-804.
  • Parker Jones, ve diğ. (2012). Where, when and why brain activation differs for bilinguals and monolinguals during picture naming and reading aloud. Cerebral Cortex, 22(4), 892-902.
  • Petitto, L. A., Berens, M. S., Kovelman, I. ve diğ. (2012). The “Perceptual Wedge Hypothesis” as the basis for bilingual babies’ phonetic processing advantage: New insights from fNIRS brain imaging. Brain and Language, 121(2), 130-143.
  • Petitto, L. A., Berens, M. S., Kovelman, I., Dubins, M. H., Jasinska, K., & Shalinsky, M. (2012). The “Perceptual Wedge Hypothesis” as the basis for bilingual babies’ phonetic processing advantage: New insights from fNIRS brain imaging. Brain and Language, 121(2), 130-143.
  • Ressel, V., Pallier, C., Ventura-Campos, N., Díaz, B., Roessler, A., Ávila, C., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2012). An effect of bilingualism on the auditory cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(47), 16597-16601.
  • Saur, D., Kreher, B. W., Schnell, S., Kümmerer, D., Kellmeyer, P., Vry, M.-S., et al. (2008). Ventral and dorsal pathways for language. PNAS, 105(46), 18035-18040.
  • Spivey, M. J., & Marian, V. (1999). Cross talk between native and second languages: Partial activation of an irrelevant lexicon. Psychological Science, 10(3), 281-284.
  • St Clair-Thompson, H. L., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Executive functions and achievements in school: Shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(4), 745-759.
  • Sundara, M., Polka, L., & Molnar, M. (2008). Development of coronal stop perception: Bilingual infants keep pace with their monolingual peers. Cognition, 108(1), 232-242.
  • Şıhanlıoğlu, Ö. (2021). An error analysis upon written expressions of Syrian late bilingual individuals in Turkey (Publication No. 658906) [Master thesis, Nevşehir Hacı Bektaş Veli Üniversitesi]. Yöktez. https://tez.yok.gov.tr/ UlusalTezMerkezi/tezSorguSonucYeni.jsp
  • Tan, L. H., Liu, H. L., Perfetti, C. A., Spinks, J. A., Fox, P. T., & Gao, J. H. (2001). The neural system underlying Chinese logograph reading. Neuroimage, 13(5), 836-846.
  • Uriel, W. (1953). Languages in contact. The Hague: Mouton, 1(1).
  • Van Heuven, W. J., Dijkstra, T., & Grainger, J. (1998). Orthographic neighborhood effects in bilingual word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 39(3), 458-483.
  • Verreyt, N., Woumans, E. V. Y., Vandelanotte, D., Szmalec, A., & Duyck, W. (2016). The influence of language-switching experience on the bilingual executive control advantage. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(1), 181-190.
  • Wagner, G., Koch, K., Reichenbach, J. R., Sauer, H., & Schlösser, R. G. (2006). The special involvement of the rostrolateral prefrontal cortex in planning abilities: an event-related fMRI study with the Tower of London paradigm. Neuropsychologia, 44(12), 2337-2347.
  • Weissberger, G. H., Gollan, T. H., Bondi, M. W., Clark, L. R., & Wierenga, C. E. (2015). Language and task switching in the bilingual brain: Bilinguals are staying, not switching, experts. Neuropsychologia, 66, 193-203.
  • Wong, B., Yin, B., & O’Brien, B. (2016). Neurolinguistics: Structure, function, and connectivity in the bilingual brain. Biomed Research International, 2016.
  • Wu, Y. J., & Thierry, G. (2010). Chinese–english bilinguals reading english hear chinese. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(22), 7646-7651.
  • Yip, V., & Matthews, S. (2007). Relative clauses in Cantonese-English bilingual children: Typological challenges and processing motivations. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 29(2), 277-300.
  • Zatorre, R. J., Belin, P., & Penhune, V. B. (2002). Structure and function of auditory cortex: Music and speech. Trends in Cognitive Science, 6(1), 37-46.
  • Zou, L., Abutalebi, J., Zinszer, B., Yan, X., Shu, H., Peng, D., & Ding, G. (2012). Second language experience modulates functional brain network for the native language production in bimodal bilinguals. NeuroImage, 62(3), 1367-1375.
There are 111 citations in total.

Details

Primary Language Turkish
Subjects Cognitive Linguistics, Child Language Acquisition, Linguistic Structures (Incl. Phonology, Morphology and Syntax), Phonetics and Speech Science
Journal Section Linguistics
Authors

Ömer Şıhanlıoğlu 0000-0002-0368-2153

Publication Date December 26, 2024
Submission Date September 20, 2024
Acceptance Date December 19, 2024
Published in Issue Year 2024 Volume: 9 Issue: 2

Cite

APA Şıhanlıoğlu, Ö. (2024). İKİDİLLİLERDE SESBİLİMSEL İŞLEMLEME VE DİL SİNİR AĞLARI. Çukurova Üniversitesi Türkoloji Araştırmaları Dergisi, 9(2), 1185-1208. https://doi.org/10.32321/cutad.1553753